Inspired by the work of psychologists who study the human face for clues that someone is telling a high-stakes lie, University at Buffalo computer scientists are exploring whether machines can also read the visual cues that give away deceit.
Results so far are promising: In a study of 40 videotaped conversations, an automated system that analyzed eye movements correctly identified whether interview subjects were lying or telling the truth 82.5 percent of the time.
That's a better accuracy rate than expert human interrogators typically achieve in lie-detection judgment experiments, says Ifeoma Nwogu (pronounced "e-fo-ma nwo-gu"), a research assistant professor at UB's Center for Unified Biometrics and Sensors (CUBS) who helped develop the system. In published results, even experienced interrogators average closer to 65 percent, Nwogu says.
"What we wanted to understand was whether there are signal changes emitted by people when they are lying, and can machines detect them? The answer was yes, and yes," Nwogu says.
The peer-reviewed research, "Lie to Me: Deceit Detection Via Online Behavioral Learning," was presented as part of the 2011 IEEE Conference on Automatic Face and Gesture Recognition.
Nwogu's colleagues on the study included CUBS scientists Nisha Bhaskaran and Venu Govindaraju, and UB communication professor Mark G. Frank, a behavioral scientist whose primary area of research has been facial expressions and deception.
In the past, Frank's attempts to automate deceit detection have used systems that analyze changes in body heat or examine a slew of involuntary facial expressions.
The automated UB system tracked a different trait — eye movement. The system employed a statistical technique to model how people moved their eyes in two distinct situations: during regular conversation, and while fielding a question designed to prompt a lie.
People whose pattern of eye movements changed between the first and second scenario were assumed to be lying, while those who maintained consistent eye movement were assumed to be telling the truth. In other words, when the critical question was asked, a strong deviation from normal eye movement patterns suggested a lie.
Previous experiments in which human judges coded facial movements found documentable differences in eye contact at times when subjects told a high-stakes lie.
What Nwogu and fellow computer scientists did was create an automated system that could verify and improve upon information used by human coders to successfully classify liars and truth tellers. The next step will be to expand the number of subjects studied and develop automated systems that analyze body language in addition to eye contact.
Nwogu says that while the sample size was small, the findings are exciting.
They suggest that computers may be able to learn enough about a person's behavior in a short time to assist with a task that challenges even experienced interrogators. The videos used in the study showed people with various skin colors, head poses, lighting and obstructions such as glasses.
This does not mean machines are ready to replace human questioners, however — only that computers can be a helpful tool in identifying liars, Nwogu says.
She notes that the technology is not foolproof: A very small percentage of subjects studied were excellent liars, maintaining their usual eye movement patterns as they lied. Also, the nature of an interrogation and interrogators' expertise can influence the effectiveness of the lie-detection method.
The videos used in the study were culled from a set of 132 that Frank recorded during a previous experiment.
In Frank's original study, 132 interview subjects were given the option to "steal" a check made out to a political party or cause they strongly opposed.
Subjects who took the check but lied about it successfully to a retired law enforcement interrogator received rewards for themselves and a group they supported; Subjects caught lying incurred a penalty: they and their group received no money, but the group they despised did. Subjects who did not steal the check faced similar punishment if judged lying, but received a smaller sum for being judged truthful.
The interrogators opened each interview by posing basic, everyday questions. Following this mundane conversation, the interrogators asked about the check. At this critical point, the monetary rewards and penalties increased the stakes of lying, creating an incentive to deceive and do it well.
In their study on automated deceit detection, Nwogu and her colleagues selected 40 videotaped interrogations.
They used the mundane beginning of each to establish what normal, baseline eye movement looked like for each subject, focusing on the rate of blinking and the frequency with which people shifted their direction of gaze.
The scientists then used their automated system to compare each subject's baseline eye movements with eye movements during the critical section of each interrogation — the point at which interrogators stopped asking everyday questions and began inquiring about the check.
If the machine detected unusual variations from baseline eye movements at this time, the researchers predicted the subject was lying.
No entries found