acm-header
Sign In

Communications of the ACM

Communications of the ACM

GSS For Presentation Support


Presentations are an ubiquitous feature of organizational life. They are a key method for achieving a clear understanding of complex ideas among large groups of people. However, there is often little time for genuine interaction among presenters and their audiences. Imagine a presentation where the entire audience jumped into a lively debate as soon as the presentation began. Imagine the presenter continued unconcerned, that everybody heard the presentation without losing the thread of the discussion in the audience. Imagine the participants could report the discussions months later with complete accuracy. With standard methods this would be impossible, yet with Group Support Systems (GSS) this scenario can be realized fairly easily.

A GSS is a suite of software tools for focusing and structuring group deliberation, while reducing the cognitive costs of communication and information access among teams making a joint cognitive effort toward a goal [3]. GSS participants simultaneously type their contributions into a network of computers. The software immediately makes all contributions available to the other participants. If the team feels it appropriate, the GSS allows for anonymous input.

A GSS may have tools for collaborative idea generation and organization, electronic polling, simultaneous document authoring, and multicriteria decision-making, among others. Each tool creates a different kind of group dynamic. One may encourage participants to diverge from customary thought patterns, while another may cause them to converge quickly on key issues. One may encourage them to contribute in great depth and detail, while another may move them to a broader, big-picture view. Here, we rethink the traditional presentation-and-discussion structure, exploring means that may enhance its value for both audiences and presenters.

Back to Top

Potential Effects of GSS for Presentation Support

A substantial amount of literature details the benefits from the use of GSS for team problem solving [4] and for nonlecture-based collaborative learning [1, 2, 5]. Several of the key benefits (see Table 1) identified in the literature may well carry over to the use of GSS in presentation environments.

Increased discussion time. Using GSS eliminates the need to divide available airtime among potential speakers because participants may contribute simultaneously. The parallel, non-oral communication channels afforded by a GSS multiply the available airtime by the number of computers available to the audience. Because they are communicating online, the participants may interact with one another during the actual presentation, which further multiplies available airtime.

Increased participation. The anonymity allowed by a GSS may reduce concerns about negative repercussions from contributing unpopular, critical, or new ideas. This, combined with increased available airtime, should lead to increased participation in discussions.

More equal participation. Since GSS provide many parallel communication channels, loud or strong personalities cannot dominate the discussion. Unlike oral discussions, the amount contributed by one person is independent of the amount contributed by others. This should lead to a more equal distribution of discussion among the group.

Permanent record of discussion. The GSS captures a permanent electronic transcript of the online discussion. Both participants and presenters can therefore access the details long after the discussion is over.

Improved feedback to presenters. With unrestricted airtime for audience members, and a permanent record of their discussion, presenters should receive more comments, and may also receive more-detailed arguments and dialectics. Because contributions can be anonymous, presenters may receive more unfiltered critical analysis of their work.

Improved learning. The GSS may also reduce attention blocking—the loss of attentiveness caused by trying to remember what one wants to say in the post-presentation discussion. Working in parallel, participants may record their ideas as soon as they occur, then return their attention to the presentation. With more discussion time, reduced attention blocking, increased participation, improved feedback, and a permanent record, GSS users may find they retain more knowledge from a presentation than people using conventional methods.

Remote and asynchronous participation. People who do not attend the presentation may still benefit by reading and contributing to online discussion after the event. However, we do not advocate replacing all face-to-face conferences and presentations with distributed online interaction. Many people find the casual conversations in the hallways and over meals to be at least as valuable as the formal content of the presentations.

Potential negative effects. Despite the anticipated benefits, we were concerned that online discussions during presentations might be a mixed blessing. Human attention resources are limited, so online discussions might distract the participants to the point where they lose the thread of the presentation. Such distractions could outweigh other benefits. Further, it is possible that online discussions could digress from the concepts of the presentation, or even devolve into flaming. The anonymity of online discussion could hinder the evolution of a social community among the participants. We therefore studied these potential negative outcomes in addition to potential benefits.

Back to Top

Field Investigations

Creating national IT policy. In 1995 we used GSS to support a group of 50 officials from 24 British Commonwealth countries at a three-day conference in Malta. They met to develop recommendations to their heads-of-state about creating national IT policies. They heard briefings from 20 nations, and participated in several evening workshops.

We arranged 25 laptop computers around a large U-shaped table in a hotel ballroom (see Figure 1). We gave a 10-minute introduction to the GSS,1 then sent the participants a stack of electronic comment cards on their screens, one for each presentation (Figure 2). At the start of each day, we encouraged the participants to enter any comments or questions that occurred to them as the presentations unfolded. All comments would be anonymous, unless the participants chose to identify themselves.

Over the three days of the conference, there were some 20 presentations, but no presentation received more than six electronic comments. At the end of each presentation, five minutes were allowed for oral discussion. At no time did anyone refer to the contents of the electronic discussion, even though this information was permanently displayed on a public screen at the front of the room (see Figure 1).

We hypothesized at first that the participants might be reluctant to use GSS under any circumstances. However, they attended evening breakout sessions where they used the GSS extensively and enthusiastically for generating, organizing and prioritizing IT policy issues. The conference organizer asserted that the hour-long evening sessions were more productive than all the daytime sessions combined. This suggested the participants were not reluctant to use GSS in general, but only during the presentations.

Informal interviews revealed that many people feared that the presenter and the other participants might regard typing as an impolite distraction. However, a critical event suggested that this fear might be unfounded. On the second afternoon, a 12-member steering committee had a strong need for a planning meeting, yet they also wanted to hear the scheduled presentations. They decided to use the GSS to hold a meeting-within-a-meeting. The committee members were in the main hall scattered among the audience at large, typing, organizing, and discussing ideas on their laptop computers.

None of the other participants showed any awareness that the online meeting had occurred. There were no glares, no pointed comments, no signs of discontent, although the steering committee meeting spanned several presentations. This suggested that despite their fears, GSS use during a presentation would not be an impolite distraction. We therefore sought ways to overcome this social misperception.

Academic conference I. In January 1996, as part of the 29th Hawaii International Conference on System Sciences (HICSS29), 43 participants attended a three-hour tutorial on business process reengineering. We placed 24 laptops around two sets of tables (Figure 3), with two public screens showing PowerPoint slides for the presentation and the contents of the electronic discussion. To overcome concerns about politeness, the presenter expressed great interest in audience feedback, and stated that he considered typing while he spoke to be both polite and desirable. However, only eight comments were submitted over the course of the three hours. Similarly low levels of participation occurred in two later 90-minute paper presentation sessions. Again, informal interviews revealed a widespread fear of rudeness.

We hypothesized that because people had never experienced using GSS during a presentation, they might not imagine how nonintrusive it could be. We also hypothesized that participants might not realize how easy the software was to use. The following day, we used the GSS for three 90-minute sessions. Each session had three paper presentations.

As each session began the moderator asked participants to use the GSS to respond to the question, "What are the most pressing research issues facing the technology-supported learning research community?" Everyone contributed an idea, then responded online to an idea contributed by someone else.

The first presenter told the group that the oral discussion following the presentations would draw ideas from the online discussion. Two subsequent speakers asked for online responses to specific questions. All others asked for critical feedback about their presentations. As soon as the first speaker began, members of the audience started typing, contributing 275 comments during the three sessions.

Participants contributed 275 comments during the three sessions, ranging from 20 to 54 per presentation. About 94% of comments were presentation-related, and there were no instances of flaming. Furthermore, during other sessions with no GSS, oral contributions to the post-presentation discussions came from no more than four people. Observations in the GSS-supported sessions showed that contributions came from all over the audience.

Our experience at HICSS29 suggested that GSS might be a useful tool for presentation support, but that our techniques needed refining. From the moderator's point of view, these three sessions were a clear success, with broad and deep communication between participants. Participants were still concerned that the speakers might regard the GSS as an impolite distraction, although feedback from the speakers revealed that the opposite might be true. For example, one speaker reported that he was nervous that he might be boring the audience until he said something that provoked a rush of typing, which he interpreted as evidence that he had engaged their interest.

During another presentation the audience argued online about an issue that seemed only tangentially related to the paper being presented. Nonetheless, the presenter was pleased by the arguments, and reported that he would incorporate them into a future version of the paper.

At the final session we used the GSS to elicit feedback about the GSS experience. From that feedback we drafted a questionnaire which we pilot tested by emailing it to 71 presenters and audience members several weeks after the conference.

A total of 21 of the 71 people responded. Although this was too small a sample to draw meaningful conclusions, some interesting patterns emerged, and we used the data to substantially improve the survey instrument. Many of the respondents to the pilot questionnaire reported feeling somewhat distracted by the GSS, and about a third of them reported that the distraction might be a problem during a presentation. After the conference we calculated that despite their active participation, the participants only used slightly less than 5% of the available person-minutes for reading and contributing to the GSS. The rest of the time they spent listening, talking, or doing other things. We suspected this relatively low consumption of person-minutes would not constitute a major distraction from the presentation, particularly since it might be offset by a reduction in attention blocking. We therefore recrafted the questionnaire so we could measure whether people in GSS-supported sessions would report more distraction than would people in sessions without GSS.

Observations of the presentation process suggested that people were more willing and able to participate in the discussion than in non-GSS supported presentations. Most of the respondents agreed that GSS encouraged participation and slightly more than half said that they were more willing to participate as a result of the availability of the technology. Most reported feeling the technology was valuable to the presenter and participants. They rated the GSS-supported sessions as more stimulating and of higher quality than unsupported sessions at the same conference. One participant, in an upbeat vein typical of others, commented "I was able to present, develop and argue my own points in the electronic discussion in much more depth than I would have in the traditional forum. I would have lost the floor before the argument was finished without the technology."

About half the respondents thought they learned more by using the GSS, but approximately one-third were undecided. Seven respondents were less than satisfied with having the GSS in the audience, and five reported they would not want to use the software for their own presentations in the future. Because the contributions were anonymous, presenters were not able to follow up with contributors. When presenters ran out of time, there was no time for oral discussion. Under these circumstances the anonymity created another concern. One participant observed: "One of the things I like about a conference is that I can figure out who is a sharp cookie and who is a blowhard. I get to know my colleagues by the way they handle themselves in the discussion time. This time I really couldn't tell."

To deal with this issue we conducted more pilot tests using the GSS with identified comments at several other conferences. The level of participation dropped immediately and substantially when comments were identified. The comments became bland and shallow, ("Nice presentation, Jim. Interesting work. Have you seen Smith's '94 paper on this subject?"). No online argumentation and no critical analysis emerged. We therefore switched back to anonymous contributions, but rigorously enforced the time boundaries of each presentation to ensure participants had sufficient time for oral discussion, and could get to know one another as they discussed the issues. This seemed to alleviate the problem.

Academic conference II. Over the following year we refined both our GSS methods and our questionnaire instrument by supporting presentations at a variety of educational, business, and government functions. We then conducted a more rigorous follow-on study at the 30th Hawaii International Conference on System Sciences (HICSS30). The study addressed three primary research questions: What effect would GSS have on participation and perceived learning?; Would the GSS be perceived as a detrimental distraction?; What effect would GSS use have on the perceived value of the presentations and discussions?

At HICSS30 we arranged 34 laptops in a configuration designed to enable participants sitting at the sides of the room to have a clearer view of the public screen (Figure 4). All GSS-supported sessions began with a brief hands-on activity that was related to the topic of the session. A moderator invited online participation at the beginning of each presentation. Most presenters added their encouragement. Participants were urged to raise key issues from the online discussions during the post-presentation discussions.

We received data from 173 participants. Of those, 73 reported having used GSS, while 70 reported they did not. Of those, 40 were from sessions where GSS were not available. Thirty people did not report on their GSS use, and so were excluded from most subsequent analysis. The 30 excluded data sets did not appear to differ significantly from other data sets.

Table 2 compares the perceptions of GSS users with those of non-GSS users based on the multi-item scales contained in the questionnaire. (All scales had an interitem reliability of 0.80 or higher).

Participation and learning. GSS users were significantly more willing to participate in the discussions than were non-GSS users, and they reported doing so at significantly higher levels (Table 2). Direct observation confirmed the reports: the participants in both the GSS-supported and standard presentations had equal opportunity to contribute to oral discussions, and did so at approximately equal rates. However, the participants who used GSS also contributed hundreds of comments to the online discussion, so their overall participation was substantially higher. Further, a much higher percentage of the audience got involved in the GSS discussion than in the oral discussion.

A total of 84% of GSS users reported that communication was easy and efficient, while only 58% of nonusers found it so. Regression analysis revealed significant linear relationships between ease-of-communication and willingness-to-participate; between willingness-to-participate and reported-participation; between willingness-to-participate and perceived-learning; and between reported-participation and perceived-learning (see Table 3).

Thus, it appears that the GSS may have accomplished its primary purpose: to increase participation and learning. But at what cost?

Distraction and digression. Overall, participants were comfortable with the degree of distraction and digression in the sessions (see Table 4). There were no differences in these perceptions based on the use or non-use of GSS (see Table 2). Only three GSS users and four non-GSS users reported negative distractions that mattered to them, too few for meaningful statistical analysis. Thus, GSS did not appear to have created widespread perceptions of undue distraction or digression.

Direct observations supported these results. As in previous investigations, there were no instances of online flaming, and nearly all of the online contributions were relevant to the presentations. Content analysis of online transcripts suggested that participants had achieved a good grasp of the key concepts in the presentations, which is further evidence the GSS did not distract them from the oral delivery of information. It is interesting to note that there appears to be a direct linear relationship between the perceived frequency of digressions and their perceived value (see Table 3). As more digressions occurred, discussions were perceived to be both more enlivening and better understood. Clearly, digressions need not diminish the value of discussions.

Overall, respondents reported positive perceptions of the value they derived from the conference sessions and the methods used in those sessions. They were also positively disposed toward the content of those sessions: the paper presentations, and the discussions (see Table 4). This suggests that the GSS enabled the groups to increase the quantity of something they valued—discussions and feedback—without reducing its quality. Many participants opted to take electronic copies of the transcripts with them at the end of each session, others downloading transcripts from the Internet. Thus the value derived from the discussion was extended beyond the walls of the presentation hall.

Back to Top

Conclusions and Recommendations

The results of these three investigations suggest that there is substantial value to be derived from using GSS to support presentation-style meetings. The expected benefits of more time for discussions, increased participation in discussions, more equal participation, and increased understanding of the presentations appear to be attainable without undue distraction or digression, and with no discernible loss of value from sessions, presentations or discussions. The methods described here are some of the simplest possible with GSS, yet they appear to be intensely useful. The GSS functions most important to the success of this study were simultaneous input, instantaneous viewing of submissions, and anonymity, yet these are only a fraction of the capability available in GSS. Future research must explore the untapped potential of this technology.

Subsequent to this investigation, the use of GSS for presentation-style meetings has become standard practice in a growing number of organizations. For example, the U.S. Navy uses this approach for its annual Flag Summit at the Naval Postgraduate School in Monterey, for a semi-annual Command Ship summit, for Federated Battle Lab Directors' meetings, and for meetings of the senior leadership team of Navy Special Warfare (the SEALS) among other purposes. These and other groups consistently report high satisfaction and benefit from their GSS experiences.

We could not have guessed at the start how important the hands-on warm-up exercise would be for overcoming the initial reluctance to type while someone else is speaking. Yet so it seems to be. The warm-up exercise need not be extensive—a single contribution and a single follow-up comment seem to be sufficient. Topics may be related to the meeting, or may be humorous asides. One moderator we know always asks, "What did you think when you first walked in and saw all these computers." The levity of the online responses often breaks the ice for the group, in addition to easing their distraction concerns. Another always asks "What do you hope to accomplish in this meeting?" The groups then refer to their responses throughout the rest of the meeting.

Further work is certainly called for, both in extending the support of presentations to different environments, and for examining different leadership techniques, different GSS tools, and their effects on group interaction and learning.

Back to Top

References

1. Brandt, S.A. and Lonsdale, M. Technology supported cooperative learning in secondary education. In Proceedings of the 29th Hawaii International Conference on System Sciences III (1996), 313–322.

2. Briggs, R.O., Ramesh, V., Romano N.C., and Latimer, J. The Exemplar project: Using group support systems to improve the learning environment. Journal of Educational Technology Systems 23, (1994), 277–287.

3. Davison, R.M. and Briggs, R.O. GSS for presentation-style meetings. In Proceedings of the 30th Hawaii International Conference on System Sciences II (1997), 430–439.

4. Nunamaker, J.F., Briggs, R.O., Mittleman, D., Vogel, D.R., and Balthazard, P. Lessons from a dozen years of group support systems research: A discussion of lab and field findings. Journal of Management Information Systems 13, 3 (1997), 163–207.

5. Shneiderman, B., Alavi, M., Norman, K., and Borkowski, E.Y. Windows of opportunity in electronic classrooms. Commun. ACM 38, 11 (Nov. 1995), 19–24.

Back to Top

Authors

Robert Davison ([email protected]) is an assistant professor at the City University of Hong Kong.

Robert O. Briggs ([email protected]) is Research Coordinator in the Center for the Management of Information at the University of Arizona, and the director of the Distributed Collaboration Group at the U.S. Navy Space and Naval Warfare Systems.

Back to Top

Footnotes

1Throughout this study, we used the Topic Commenter tool from GroupSystems for Windows (v.1.1), developed by researchers at the University of Arizona and commercialized by Ventana Corporation.

Back to Top

Figures

F1Figure 1. British Commonwealth Secretariat room layout.

F2Figure 2. The GSS used in the British Commonwealth Secretariat conference. (Note the stack of electronic comment cards, one for each presentation: one is open, revealing contributions from the audience.)

F3Figure 3. HICSS29 room layout.

F4Figure 4. HICSS30 room layout.

Back to Top

Tables

T1Table 1. Potential effects of GSS on presentation-style meetings.

T2Table 2. Perceptions of GSS and Non-GSS users. (A higher value indicates a more positive perception.)

T3Table 3. Relationships among key perceptions. (Linear regression analyses.)

T4Table 4. Overall perceptions of distraction and digression. (Higher means indicate a more-favorable perception; one-sample t-tests of difference-from-neutral.)

Back to top


©2000 ACM  0002-0782/00/0900  $5.00

Permission to make digital or hard copies of all or part of this work for personal or classroom use is granted without fee provided that copies are not made or distributed for profit or commercial advantage and that copies bear this notice and the full citation on the first page. To copy otherwise, to republish, to post on servers or to redistribute to lists, requires prior specific permission and/or a fee.

The Digital Library is published by the Association for Computing Machinery. Copyright © 2000 ACM, Inc.


 

No entries found